[IAEP] reconstructed maths

Bill Kerr billkerr at gmail.com
Sun Jul 27 02:11:25 EDT 2008


alan kay:
An interesting paper by one of your countrymen that Mark Guzdial pointed me
to (After the Gold Rush: Toward Sustainable Scholarship in Computing, by
Raymond Lister, University of Technology, Sydney, Australia --
http://crpit.com/confpapers/CRPITV78Lister.pdf ) shows some of the
difficulties of dealing with this very complex area. I don't know quite how
to do justice to a counter argument in a very short space here ... (snip)

IMO the Raymond Lister is an example of a teacher seriously trying to
improve his interaction with his students but along the way some things
become misrepresented, in particular, constructivism, due to uncritical
assimilation of different theories, in this case, Cognitive Load Theory.
Since the issue of Cognitive Load Theory keeps coming up as an alleged
refutation of constructivism I wrote a detailed response to the Lister paper
on my blog:
http://billkerr2.blogspot.com/2008/07/raymond-listers-paper.html


On Wed, Jul 23, 2008 at 6:34 AM, Alan Kay <alan.nemo at yahoo.com> wrote:

> Hi Bill --
>
> Suppose we take as a premise that the following results of surveys over the
> last 20 years were gathered well enough to form a real generalization:
> - only 20% of American adults can read a well written important ideas essay
> (they used Tom Paine's "Common Sense"), and understand it well enough to
> discuss it, write about it, criticize it, advocate it, etc. (National
> Literacy Foundation)
> - only 5% of American adults are "literate/fluent" enough in math & science
> to deal with mainstream ideas, have an extended conversation with a
> mathematician or scientist, be operational enough to put a mathematical map
> on a set of ideas and do something with them, etc.
>
> If we weed a few more artifacts out of this survey (and the surveyors did
> some of this already -- such as not counting those who were not educated in
> American schools, etc.), then we can pretty reasonably conclude that the
> schemes of education employed in the US have failed (miserably) to meet the
> goals of education in America.
>
> The reason I say "suppose ... premise" above is that we have to be very
> careful about "scientific studies" outside of "science", because sometimes
> only the trappings and not the substance of "science" remains. In the above
> case, it looks as though (especially) the NLF did a comprehensive job of
> sampling and concluding.
>
> However, this is not often the case in most studies of educational methods
> and results. And, it is very difficult to separate out and test any method
> from the testing that is also going on of evaluating how well randomly
> chosen teachers in America can teach anything in any style. I would posit
> that trying to do this in single trials is essentially intractable.
>
> I've mentioned before that just validating a piece of our curriculum
> requires three years of doing it with a teacher in a classroom before enough
> of the artifacts and distracters can be nudged out to get even a qualitative
> judgment. (In our case, this has very often been a "no, this is not a good
> way to approach this that will get more than 90% of the children above a
> real fluency threshold" -- i.e. failure.) No-one wants to pay for these
> extra years, so we use our discretionary research budget to support the
> extra costs and time.
>
> A much more important kind of investigation in education is a "transfer
> study", which is all about whether enough of both practical and abstract
> understanding is retained and operationalized enough to be applied by the
> learner in later contexts (both related to the original learning and in
> areas where there could be very fruitful analogies). For example, in the 70s
> Adele Goldberg and I designed an extensive transfer study to see if
> "powerful ideas in programming/active-math" could be foundations for more
> powerful learning and thinking in other fields (we chose major parts of
> Biology). Because of the kind of setups and testing needed, we thought that
> at least 7 years should be devoted to this. For example, there were several
> overlapping three year implementations in the programming/active-math ideas,
> and the kind of testing we had already been doing, and then there would have
> to be another series of these in the later experimental and control classes
> when the children started learning about Biology.
>
> Needless to say NSF turned this down flat, and turned down several
> subsequent requests we made.
>
> However, even if they had funded the study, we realized that it would be
> adding more of the largest problem of doing anything in a school with math
> or science, which is working with teachers who don't remotely understand
> their subjects -- and (even in the case of reading and writing) don't do the
> activities with the children (when was the last time you saw a 5th grade
> teacher assign a composition to the students and then let the students pick
> a topic for them and write an essay along with the students?). (Actually,
> given the excellence of your blog, you might be an exception!)
>
> This, along with many other reasons, is why I don't worry about the "C"
> word or the "D" word, or any other simple scheme. As Marvin Minsky once
> pointed out, every educational method works for some students. This is
> because another deeply important factor is that children in a single
> classroom exhibit a wide variation in motivations, knowledge, skills,
> maturity and "wiring". Different children need different approaches. A
> classroom is a tough place to learn anything (as an orchestra is a tough
> place to learn how to play an instrument). The US factory approach to
> education was hoping for economies of scales via method, but it forgot that
> it wasn't about just turning out Model-T's, but every kind and variation of
> vehicle using every kind and variation of materials and design.
>
> Long (very long) ago I was a professional musician (jazz guitar) and also
> taught guitar for a few years. The basics for musical learning are rather
> similar to sports learning, and they involve rather different approaches and
> mixes of processes than in formal schools. (Of course, they might be so
> different from learning math that no analogies will hold -- but let's
> pretend that they aren't so different.)
>
> The goal in music-sports is fluent playing. It is not known how to do this
> without having the learners undergo a lot of "doing of playing". However,
> there is not a lot of discovery to be done early on that is going to help
> and not hinder later on (i.e. most ideas are mediocre down to bad -- this is
> why good ideas are so rare and precious). But, as Tim Gallwey the great
> tennis teacher says, "The problem with most theories of learning is that the
> parts of your body that need to learn, don't understand English!" Saying it
> a different way, the parts of our mind that do understand natural language,
> aren't often able to do other subjects well. We can see this is also true
> for math and science -- otherwise we could just write the best expositional
> essay on each subject (called "great books") and just get the learners to
> read them! And, imagine how easy it would be to teach teen-agers to drive a
> car! Obviously, other elements are vital.
>
> If we combine a few ideas -- e.g. discovery is really difficult, it's hard
> to learn via language, we have limited capacity for dealing with ideas at
> one time (7+-2 according to George Miller), etc. -- then we can see that
> Jerry Bruner's notion of "scaffolding" starts to come front and center as a
> way to devise strategies for learning sequences. For example, a teacher can
> set things up so that only a few degrees of freedom remain, and now there is
> a much higher chance of actual discovery, or homing in on what is best to
> concentrate on. This is done all the time in music-sports.
>
> For example, Ted Williams introduced the batting tee into professional
> baseball and was pooh-poohed for "silly, unmanly, etc.". But he was the
> greatest hitter of his day (and one of the greatest of all time) so
> gradually others began to surreptitiously practice. His idea was that it was
> almost impossible even getting the muscular feeling and memory for a level
> swing if you are going against a moving target of "round thing against round
> thing". Now the batting tee is found in every training facility for all
> levels of baseball and there is even a league for very young players.
>
> Scaffolding has to be carefully vetted. For example, short skis really seem
> to work for learning beginning skiing, but putting frets on a violin doesn't
> (even though they seem to help in the beginning - then they hurt badly).
> However, "multiperson African Drumming" really does help all aspects of
> music learning, including classical music.
>
> Showing" often helps. If you can't feel the phrasing of a musical sequence,
> sometimes it's just best for the teacher to play various phrasings to be
> judged. Or to get you to watch them serve (the flip side of this is that the
> top tennis pros have rather different strokes and serves -- i.e. personal
> wirings and idiosyncrasies have to be tolerated -- it is very difficult to
> learn exactly what someone else does -- but one can learn "just as well
> though a little differently").
>
> This hurts badly in school when the teachers don't know enough math or
> science to be flexible about perspectives, etc. We would be surprised if our
> music or tennis teacher weren't fluent and refused to play with us (for one
> thing, that's the best way for them to assess where their students are) --
> we would doubtless drop a "non-doing" teacher. But the opposite is
> egregiously true for most school teachers, most are not and have never been
> practitioners. However, we only see a few parents take their kids out of
> public school for such reasons.
>
> We could well imagine that one form of instruction might score better than
> another if teachers are not up to snuff (however, as mentioned above, the
> "better" is not nearly good enough to get the eventual American adults above
> any reasonable threshold). If we are going for "evidence" and "scientific
> evaluation", then we have to include getting to real thresholds, not just
> relative differences. Here, all methods currently fail -- and probably will
> until better conceptions and thresholds are created for teachers.
>
> Gallwey again: "You still have to hit thousands of balls to learn tennis,
> the difference is what you are thinking about and how you are focusing while
> doing".
>
> This is as good a key to progress as any.
>
> An interesting paper by one of your countrymen that Mark Guzdial pointed me
> to (After the Gold Rush: Toward Sustainable Scholarship in Computing, by
> Raymond Lister, University of Technology, Sydney, Australia --
> http://crpit.com/confpapers/CRPITV78Lister.pdf ) shows some of the
> difficulties of dealing with this very complex area. I don't know quite how
> to do justice to a counter argument in a very short space here, but I think
> there are real parallels with what happens with learning programming (he
> gives his POV as a college teacher of programming) to what happens with
> learning music, sports, and even driving a car, if the learners don't do
> enough of the actual processes. For example, he makes the (to me) astounding
> statement that:
>
> I taught a first semester programming subject, where the final exam
> consisted entirely of multiple-choice questions (Lister & Leaney, 2003a&b;
> Lister, 2005). I adopted that style of exam because it was clear to me
> that many students could not write code by the end of first semester, and
> I was tired of setting and marking exams where I pretended that students
> could write code.
>
> They couldn't write code at the end of a semester? Was the course about
> anything else? What kind of grades could he be giving? "Air guitar" grades
> for "air programming"?
>
> Much other of interest will be found herein.
>
> There aren't enough details in the paper to comment on his teaching style
> or to guess why his students couldn't program at the end of a semester.
> (This is not the only such story that has been written up over the years.)
> In some of the latter cases, I knew some of the instructors and they were
> not dunderheads by any means. So we could certainly give Mr. Lister the
> benefit of the doubt, and wonder instead about the processes in his class
> and in universities in general.
>
> Now, if we do the (so far unwarranted) act of substituting music, sports or
> even driving a car, we might guess that the main reason the students were in
> this unhappy state at the end of a semester is because of the pace, depth
> and amount (if not also the nature) of the doing experience.
>
> Another unwarranted comparison is to the way programmers were created in
> the military services in the 50s and 60s. Virtually all participants were
> enlisted personnel without college educations and some without high school.
> Programming was needed, but was not glamorous enough to be within the ken of
> the college educated officers.
>
> Prospects (in this case, the Air Force) were given a short aptitude test
> (about 45 minutes) made up by IBM that essentially assessed interests and
> latent abilities in patterns of various kinds. Only people who got through
> this went to the next state -- which was a one week wall-to-wall (40 hours
> of class plus lots of assignments) of instruction in how to program a
> computer. This was also conducted by IBM, and in my memory was just about
> perfect in the balance of description, advice, examples, and many doings
> with one's own code. (I had similar favorable impressions with the rest of
> the training I got while in the military -- the only thing left out was
> "education", meaning that "theory" was scant -- every other aspect could not
> have been better thought through and presented.)
>
> One hectic week later, one knew the machine code and assembler and could
> write many programs for the real computer that was back on base, and that
> was what we did to other's goals for several months. This was intensive and
> literally "on the job training". One thing that people find unusual today,
> was that not only was there no interactive programming (punched cards were
> submitted for a batch run), but one was allowed a maximum of five minutes
> actual contact each day, not with the machine, but via an operator who ran
> the machine, could punch in addresses, etc. One had one's listing draped
> over the card reader and was kept well away from the console. Basically, the
> only way you could get a program to run was to have it be "almost perfect"
> before testing. This was accomplished via another developed skill called
> "desk checking" (Don Knuth attributes his facility with programming to this
> quaint process as well.)
>
> Then there was another intensive week of wall-to-wall "Advanced
> Programming" in which one learned a little more architecture and how to use
> the extensive macro facility in the assembler, etc. I will only compare the
> first intensive week and month or so which resulted in real programming
> skills to Lister's very different experience in university.
>
> The point here is that the armed services scheme had almost no failures,
> everyone who went through it was successful. The instructors weren't any
> better than the college professors, but the process really was. And the
> goals were very different. There wasn't any class to pass, no multiple
> choice tests to take, no grading on the curve, only a few hours of "lecture"
> (and just when needed), and (no small matter) there was nothing to do but to
> learn programming that "semester". The basic idea here in 1961 (I think) was
> that if you can think a little, then a "summer music camp" approach is the
> best way to really get going on something. If you can't think a little (play
> a musical instrument a little) then you should get across this threshold and
> then go to summer camp.
>
> (Way afield, CMU did something quite similar and very wonderful and
> successful for their incoming CS grad students wrt CS at CMU.)
>
> Again, this successful scheme doesn't necessarily generalize to every
> subject. But it's strong enough to be worth considering in areas where
> "doing skills" are an important part of the subject. (One problem with
> "math" in the US is that it isn't actually "math" but only simple
> calculation skills. This isn't enough to help with actual math thinking
> (which is a special skill all its own that can indeed be taught, but isn't.)
>
> An important aspect of this approach is that it nicely avoids having to
> categorize methods: it is really about a somewhat vague but readily
> understandable approach in which the only real goal is to help the learner
> achieve fluency in "something that is done". We can safely ignore the C and
> D words in favor of the E and T words (or the M and S words).
>
> Best wishes,
>
> Alan
>
> ----- Original Message ----
> From: Bill Kerr <billkerr at gmail.com>
> To: Albert Cahalan <acahalan at gmail.com>
> Cc: its.an.education.project at tema.lo-res.org
> Sent: Monday, July 21, 2008 7:08:49 PM
> Subject: Re: [IAEP] reconstructed maths
>
> On Thu, Jul 17, 2008 at 7:16 PM, Albert Cahalan <acahalan at gmail.com>
> wrote:
>
>> Alan Kay writes:
>>
>> > Similarly, from the POV of a former guitar teacher and player,
>> > "playing guitar" has a threshold that excludes "air guitar",
>> > "Guitar Hero", certain kinds of noise, and "not enough fluency
>> > to make music yet". We can use "air guitar" as a metaphor for
>> > (a) taking such a small subset of an activity that only some
>> > form and essentially no important content is being done, and
>> > (b) for using form over content to fool ourselves that we are
>> > "players" and "part of the club".
>>
>> That works. I also like the term "math appreciation".
>> Like a music appreciation course, it doesn't get you
>> to be competent.
>>
>> > Two of Seymour Papert's most important insights about above
>> > threshold math-with-computers for children were to (a) find and
>> > use the real mathematical thinking that children could do at each
>> > stage of development, and (b) to both pick from the large body of
>> > existing mathematics and to invent new mathematics that embody the
>> > most "powerful ideas" that humans have come up with. One of many
>> > such examples is how to use the children's ability to add (and to
>> > think additively) and to physically move their bodies to make for
>> > them a powerful and valid version of Gauss' Differential Geometry
>> > which covers some of the most important parts of vector calculus
>> > in a way easily learnable by children.
>>
>> I do believe that many children can learn vector calculus, and that
>> this might have some value. However...
>>
>> When you put the cart before the horse, trying to skip all of the
>> arithmetic and such, you're teaching math appreciation. It's air math.
>>
>> An actual bake-off has been conducted. It was the largest educational
>> study ever done, covering 79000 children in 180 communities. I'm sure
>> you've heard of it: Project Follow Through.
>>
>> In that study, Direct Instruction (sage on the stage) trounced all
>> other programs in multiple ways. More here:
>>
>> http://www.jefflindsay.com/EducData.shtml
>> http://www.illinoisloop.org/oswegomath.html
>> http://www.heartland.org/Article.cfm?artId=19790
>>
>> That last article has a lovely graph of the results.
>>
>> One may question the ethics of such an experiment, since it does in
>> fact involve experimenting on children in a life-effecting way.
>> I see no alternative though, so I'm glad it was done. Now it is time
>> to accept the results, eat some humble pie as required, and teach
>> children math.
>>
>> It is unethical to endlessly repeat the experiment, hoping that you
>> will somehow get results that support a well-loved hypothesis.
>>
>> Vector calculus is a fine subject for children. Set high expectations
>> for daily progress, eliminate distractions, and soon enough most kids
>> will reach vector calculus. (for real, not vector calculus appreciation)
>>
>> High expectations go something like this:
>>
>> multi-digit add/subtract with traditional procedure: 1st year
>> multiplication table memorized: 1st year
>> long division: 2nd year
>> 2-step word problems: 2nd year
>> 4 basic operations on fractions: 3rd year
>> 4-step word problems: 4th year
>> order of operations: 4th year
>> algebra (with proofs): 5th year
>> geometry (with proofs) and trig: 6th year
>> regular calculus: 7th year
>> vector calculus: 8th year
>>
>> It's doable, but you won't get there if you waste time or if you use
>> educational methods that are proven to be horrible.
>>
>> In case anybody wants to look at current curriculum that work:
>>
>> The two best math programs, unfortunately subject to copyright, are
>> Saxon Math and Singapore Math. Saxon Math is better for the slower
>> students, particularly if students are missing school or transferring
>> in from places that use a different math program. Singapore Math is
>> better for the faster students. Both are available for purchase.
>
>
>
> hi albert,
>
> thanks for raising an issue sharply that does need to be discussed
>
> misunderstandings and misrepresentations aside, I'd raise this point in
> response - your assumption is that a large scale study is more important
> than the individual research and findings of one person
>
> I don't see why this assumption should necessarily be true - ie.
> historically it has been shown many times that lone individuals or small
> groups have turned out to be correct and the predominant or mainstream way
> of doing things has eventually been displaced - that is the nature of
> scientific revolutions
>
> it could be that whole systems have been built and maintained for
> generations on principles of direct instruction - that various challenges to
> this have arisen and been trialled, some good, some  not so good - but
> throughout this process the predominant form of teaching has remained direct
> instruction
>
> it seems to me that in a system that has evolved in that way, that due to
> forces of inertia and group think mainstream studies would tend to show that
> mainstream ways of doing things are the "best way"
>
> Piaget did many studies and wrote many books and papers based on the study
> of 3 children - that does not in itself make him wrong. He might be wrong
> but I can see many advantages of doing in depth studies based on a small
> group.
>
> I would like to discuss this issue more, just raising it here in simple
> form --> minority views are not wrong because they are minority views
>
> Of course your challenge still applies as a practical issue for those who
> want to go beyond direct instruction at least in some respects
>
> cheers,
> - Bill
>
>


-- 
Bill Kerr
http://billkerr2.blogspot.com/
-------------- next part --------------
An HTML attachment was scrubbed...
URL: http://lists.sugarlabs.org/archive/iaep/attachments/20080727/cc952a7c/attachment-0001.htm 


More information about the IAEP mailing list